Derealization: Difference between revisions
Appearance
Xinreality (talk | contribs) No edit summary |
Xinreality (talk | contribs) No edit summary |
||
(4 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{see also|Terms|Non-technical Terms | {{see also|Terms|Non-technical Terms}} | ||
[[Derealization]] ('''DR''') is a form of [[dissociation]] in which the | [[Derealization]] ('''DR''') is a form of [[dissociation]] in which the '''external world feels unreal, dream-like, foggy, artificial, distant, or visually distorted'''.<ref name="DSM5">American Psychiatric Association. (2013). ''Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders'' (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.</ref> Although surroundings may appear "flat", "cartoonish", or "as if seen through glass", '''reality-testing remains intact''': the sufferer knows the environment itself has not actually changed. Derealization often co-occurs with [[depersonalization]] (detachment from one’s self); persistent or recurrent episodes of either, when clinically distressing, are diagnosed as [[Depersonalization/Derealization Disorder]] (DPDR).<ref name="Simeon2004">Simeon, D. (2004). Depersonalisation disorder: a contemporary overview. ''CNS Drugs'', 18 (6), 343-354. https://doi.org/10.2165/00023210-200418060-00002</ref> | ||
==Clinical definition and overview== | ==Clinical definition and overview== | ||
* | *'''Core symptoms''' include a sense that people or objects are artificial, colour-less, two-dimensional, or remote; time may seem sped up or slowed; familiar places can appear strangely foreign.<ref name="Hunter2004">Hunter, E. C., Sierra, M., & David, A. S. (2004). The epidemiology of depersonalisation and derealisation. ''Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology'', 39 (1), 9-18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-004-0701-4</ref> | ||
* | *'''Prevalence.''' Transient DR/DP episodes occur in '''26-74 %''' of the general population, but chronic DPDR affects roughly '''1-2 %'''.<ref name="Hunter2004" /><ref>Y, J., Millman, L. S., David, A. S., & Hunter, E. (2023). The prevalence of depersonalization-derealization disorder: a systematic review. ''Journal of Trauma & Dissociation'', 24 (1), 8-41. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2022.2079796</ref> | ||
===Symptoms and characteristics=== | ===Symptoms and characteristics=== | ||
Line 12: | Line 12: | ||
| Perceptual || The world seems foggy, muted, too sharp, [[macropsia]]/[[micropsia]], muted or echoing sounds | | Perceptual || The world seems foggy, muted, too sharp, [[macropsia]]/[[micropsia]], muted or echoing sounds | ||
|- | |- | ||
| Cognitive || Feeling | | Cognitive || Feeling "in a film", heightened [[self-observation]], doubts about the reality of memories | ||
|- | |- | ||
| Affective || Emotional numbing (“de-affectualisation”), blunted responses to normally salient stimuli | | Affective || Emotional numbing (“de-affectualisation”), blunted responses to normally salient stimuli | ||
Line 23: | Line 23: | ||
: Abnormal fronto-limbic connectivity; hypo-activity of the [[amygdala]] and [[insula]]; disturbances in predictive-coding networks.<ref>Gatus, A., Jamieson, G., & Stevenson, B. (2022). Past and future explanations for DPDR: a role for predictive coding. ''Frontiers in Human Neuroscience'', 16, 813300.</ref> | : Abnormal fronto-limbic connectivity; hypo-activity of the [[amygdala]] and [[insula]]; disturbances in predictive-coding networks.<ref>Gatus, A., Jamieson, G., & Stevenson, B. (2022). Past and future explanations for DPDR: a role for predictive coding. ''Frontiers in Human Neuroscience'', 16, 813300.</ref> | ||
;Substance-related | ;Substance-related | ||
: Cannabis, hallucinogens ( | : Cannabis, hallucinogens (for example LSD, psilocybin), [[ketamine]], MDMA, stimulant intoxication, or benzodiazepine withdrawal can precipitate DR.<ref>Simeon, D., Kozin, D., Segal, K., & Lerch, B. (2009). Is depersonalization disorder initiated by illicit drug use any different? ''Journal of Clinical Psychiatry'', 70 (10), 1358-1364.</ref> | ||
==Neurological mechanisms== | ==Neurological mechanisms== | ||
Neuro-imaging studies reveal a | Neuro-imaging studies reveal a '''fronto-limbic "decoupling"''': hyper-activation of pre-frontal regions suppresses limbic emotional circuits, yielding detachment from affective salience.<ref>Sierra, M. & David, A. S. (2011). Depersonalization: a selective impairment of self-awareness. ''Consciousness & Cognition'', 20 (1), 99-108.</ref> Reduced insular activity may underlie impaired [[interoception]], while alterations in visual association cortices correlate with perceptual distortions.<ref>Adler, J. et al. (2014). Altered orientation of spatial attention in DPDR. ''Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging'', 216 (2), 230-235.</ref> | ||
==Derealization and immersive technology (VR/AR)== | ==Derealization and immersive technology (VR/AR)== | ||
Immersive [[virtual reality]] (VR) and [[augmented reality]] (AR) deliberately alter multisensory input, raising concern that they may | Immersive [[virtual reality]] (VR) and [[augmented reality]] (AR) deliberately alter multisensory input, raising concern that they may '''induce transient or persistent DR/DP symptoms'''. | ||
===Induced dissociative symptoms=== | ===Induced dissociative symptoms=== | ||
A pivotal study from the Fernand-Seguin Research Center reported significant increases in DR/DP and reduced sense of [[presence]] after VR exposure.<ref name="Aardema2010">Aardema, F., O'Connor, K., Côté, S., & Taillon, A. (2010). Virtual reality induces dissociation and lowers sense of presence in objective reality. ''Cyberpsychology, Behavior, & Social Networking'', 13 (4), 429-435. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0164</ref> More recent RCTs and surveys confirm that | A pivotal study from the Fernand-Seguin Research Center reported significant increases in DR/DP and reduced sense of [[presence]] after VR exposure.<ref name="Aardema2010">Aardema, F., O'Connor, K., Côté, S., & Taillon, A. (2010). Virtual reality induces dissociation and lowers sense of presence in objective reality. ''Cyberpsychology, Behavior, & Social Networking'', 13 (4), 429-435. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0164</ref> More recent RCTs and surveys confirm that '''VR can acutely elevate DR scores''', yet effects typically resolve within minutes to hours and rarely persist.<ref>Braun, N. et al. (2022). Virtual reality induces depersonalization and derealization: a longitudinal RCT. ''Computers in Human Behavior'', 131, 107233.</ref><ref>Barreda-Ángeles, M., & Hartmann, T. (2023). Experiences of DP/DR among users of VR applications: a cross-sectional survey. ''Cyberpsychology, Behavior, & Social Networking'', 26 (1), 22-27.</ref> | ||
===Proposed mechanisms=== | ===Proposed mechanisms=== | ||
* '''[[Sensory conflict]]''' between visual and vestibular/proprioceptive inputs.<ref>Keshavarz, B. et al. (2014). Vection and motion sickness: higher susceptibility in women. ''Perception'', 43 (ECVP Suppl.), 107.</ref> | * '''[[Sensory conflict]]''' between visual and vestibular/proprioceptive inputs.<ref>Keshavarz, B. et al. (2014). Vection and motion sickness: higher susceptibility in women. ''Perception'', 43 (ECVP Suppl.), 107.</ref> | ||
* High '''immersion / presence shift''', producing a temporary imbalance favouring virtual over physical cues.<ref>Slater, M., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2016). Enhancing our lives with immersive VR. ''Frontiers in Robotics & AI'', 3, 74.</ref> | * High '''[[immersion]] / [[presence shift]]''', producing a temporary imbalance favouring virtual over physical cues.<ref>Slater, M., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2016). Enhancing our lives with immersive VR. ''Frontiers in Robotics & AI'', 3, 74.</ref> | ||
* '''Boundary blurring''' when hyper-realistic graphics or prolonged sessions weaken reality markers.<ref name="Madary2016">Madary, M., & Metzinger, T. (2016). Real virtuality: a code of ethical conduct. ''Frontiers in Robotics & AI'', 3, 3.</ref> | * '''Boundary blurring''' when hyper-realistic graphics or prolonged sessions weaken reality markers.<ref name="Madary2016">Madary, M., & Metzinger, T. (2016). Real virtuality: a code of ethical conduct. ''Frontiers in Robotics & AI'', 3, 3.</ref> | ||
Line 51: | Line 51: | ||
===Prevention and management strategies=== | ===Prevention and management strategies=== | ||
* | *'''Session design:''' build-in breaks every 20-30 min; gradual onboarding for new users. | ||
* | *'''Grounding techniques:''' post-VR mindfulness, tactile engagement, or brief exercise help re-establish physical-world presence. | ||
* | *'''User warnings:''' advise individuals with DPDR, severe [[anxiety]], or trauma histories to proceed cautiously.<ref name="Madary2016" /> | ||
* | *'''Clinical monitoring:''' therapists using VR exposure should screen for dissociative side-effects.<ref>Maples-Keller, J. L. et al. (2017). The use of VR in treatment of anxiety & psychiatric disorders. ''Harvard Review of Psychiatry'', 25 (3), 103-113.</ref> | ||
===Therapeutic applications=== | ===Therapeutic applications=== | ||
Paradoxically, controlled VR has been investigated to | Paradoxically, controlled VR has been investigated to '''treat''' dissociative conditions, for example reality, discrimination training or interoceptive biofeedback to enhance bodily awareness.<ref>Opriş, D. et al. (2012). VR exposure therapy in anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. ''Depression & Anxiety'', 29 (2), 85-93.</ref> | ||
===Ethical considerations=== | ===Ethical considerations=== |